Brief literature review about a topic related to autism

Assignment Help Other Subject
Reference no: EM131196485

The articles used have to be peer reviewed articles. Include citations and use APA guidelines.

Below is the topic, at the bottom an example of what the teacher wants. The topic has to be about a specific thing about autism.

Writing will be a major component of this course, so let's practice!

For this assignment, complete a brief literature review (no more than 5 pages of text) about a topic related to autism. You may choose any topic you wish related to autism, but be specific. A literature review is a summary of research on a particular topic. The goal is to interpret and integrate information from research articles.

EXAMPLE BELOW

Literature Review Example

Writing a Short Literature Review William Ashton, Ph.D.
York College, CUNY

A student began a short literature review on the stigma of the mentally ill and perceptions of dangerousness. Working through PsychArticles she found three likely articles. When she read each, she wrote a paragraph description of each:

Alexander, L.A., & Link, B.G. (2003). The impact of contact on stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental illness. Journal of Mental Health, 12, 271-289.

Alexander and Link (2003) examined the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant's own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact - with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place - with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette.

Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, P., Uphoff-Wasowski, K., White, K., & Kubiak, M.A. (2002). Challenging two mental illness stigmas: Personality responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 293-309.

Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies to investigate the strength of the theoretical relationship between stigma and personality responsibility, and stigma and dangerousness. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill.

In their first study, Corrigan etal. (2002), administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or a mental patient. During the meetings, either myths about the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results, contact with mental patients produced stronger results.

Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tuch, S. A. (2000). Of fear and loathing: The role of ‘disturbing behavior' labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 208-223.

Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of the targets' behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target's perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants' sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles).

Next she decided upon the order of the paragraphs in the paper:
Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of the targets' behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target's perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants' sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles).

Alexander and Link (2003) examined the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant's own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact - with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place - with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette.

Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies to investigate the strength of the theoretical relationship between stigma and personality responsibility, and stigma and dangerousness. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill.

In their first study, Corrigan etal. (2002), administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or a mental patient. During the meetings, either myths about the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results, contact with mental patients produced stronger results.

She carefully chose the order of the paragraphs so she could talk about:

(1) that people respond to the mentally ill with fear and rejection,

(2) contact reduces both rejection and fear and

(3) how to best arrange the contact to reduce stigma.

Now she added introductory and concluding sentences, paragraph hooks and short transition paragraphs to help the flow of ideas.

Regarding the mentally ill, it appears that people respond to the mentally ill with feelings of fear and rejection. Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000) examined the effects of descriptions of the targets' behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target's perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants' sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for theschizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles). Thus, a common respond to the mentally ill are rejection and fear of violence.

While, based upon research, the common response to a mentally ill person is to fear violence, diagnosed mental patients commit violence at the same rates as non-diagnosed people (Martin, et al., 2000). Public perceptions may not match reality due to the public's lack of contact with the mentally ill.

Alexander and Link (2003) examined contact with the mentally ill and the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant's own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact - with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place - with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette. Thus, according to Alexander and Link (2003), any contact with the mentally ill is associated with reduced fear and rejection. However, since this study was observational in nature, we cannot know if contact reduces fear or having lower fear increased contact.

Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies examining the causal processes in contact, fear and rejection. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions. In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, beliefs about personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill.

In their first study, Corrigan et al. (2002) administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data. Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or with a mental patient. During the meetings, myths about the personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results regarding fear and rejection, contact with mental patients produced stronger results. Thus, Corrigan et al. demonstrated that contact causes less rejection and fear.

In the final stage, she needs to write an introductory and concluding paragraph. She wrote the concluding paragraph first. In this paragraph she needs to overview the paper and make a conclusion.

It appears that the mentally ill are rejected because of the public's fear of the mentally ill. At least one-third of the people sampled in one study said that they would both reject socially and fear violence from someone displaying behaviors associated with different mentally illnesses. Other research discovered that this rejection is associated to lack of contact with the mentally ill and that as contact increased, fear of the mentally ill decreased. The direction of the relationship between fear and rejection seems to be that fear (possibly based upon myths about mental illness) causes rejection. Taken as a whole, it appears that exposing these myths as myths increases the acceptance of the mentally ill and that staged contact with a mentally person to expose myths has an even more powerful effect.

Now she needs to say something about the research methods.
Caution must be advised, though; Martin et al.'s (2002) and Alexander and Link's (2003) studies and the first study of Corrigan et al. (2002) were based upon paper and pencil methodologies. And while Corrigan et al.'s (2002) second study involved staged presentations, it was conducted in a college setting with a college sample. Future research should replicate these findings in more natural settings with different populations.

The student then brought her draft to me. After I read it I asked her about the first sentence of her conclusion. I asked her if she could phrase it as a clear and strong statement. She did:

The rejection of the mentally ill is caused by the public's belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce rejection.

Now she needs to write the Introduction. With an introduction, begin broad and narrow down to the thesis statement. The thesis statement is the last sentence in the introduction and the first sentence in the conclusion.

The mentally ill face a multitude of challenges. One of those challenges is the stigmatization they face. Stigmatization is social rejection; they are rejected by people because of the label they carry or that their behaviors clearly indicate that they belong to a certain labeled group. Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the public's belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization.

Here's how her whole paper looked:

Myths of violence and the stigma of mental illness
Suzie Student
York College, CUNY
Myths of violence 1

1 For the text of an APA article, use Courier or New Times Roman font at 12 pts.

2 Before the page number is the Page Header (sometimes called, Key Words) the first few words of your title.

Myths of violence and the stigma of mental illness :

The mentally ill face a multitude of challenges. One of those challenges is the stigmatization they face.

Stigmatization is social rejection; those stigmatized are rejected by people because of the label they carry or that their behaviors clearly indicate that they belong to a certain labeled group. Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the public's belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization.

Regarding the mentally ill, it appears that people respond to the mentally ill with feelings of fear and rejection. Martin, Pescosolido & Tuch (2000)4 examined the effects of descriptions of the targets' behavior, causal attributions about the source of the behavior, the target's perceived dangerousness, labeling and participants' sociodemographic characteristics. Twenty percent of the participants labeled a target described with depressed symptoms as having a mental illness (as compared with 54% for those described with schizophrenic symptoms or 1% with normal troubles); 37% would be unwilling to interact with the depressed person (48% for the schizophrenic and 21% for normal troubles); and 33% felt that the depressed person would do violence to others (61% for the 3 before the text begins, repeat the title, centered.

Myths of violence 2

schizophrenic and 17% for the normal troubles). Thus, a common respond to the mentally ill are rejection and fear of violence.

While, based upon research, the common response to a mentally ill person is to fear violence, diagnosed mental patients commit violence at the same rates as non-diagnosed people (Martin, et al., 2000)5. Public perceptions may not match reality due to the public's lack of contact with the mentally ill.

Alexander and Link (2003) examined contact with the mentally ill and the stigma of mental illness, perceptions of dangerousness and social distance in a telephone survey. They found that, as a participant's own life contact with mentally ill individuals increased, participants were both less likely to perceive a target mentally ill individual in a vignette as physically dangerous and less likely to desire social distance
from the target. This relationship remained after controlling for demographic and confound variables, such as gender, ethnicity, education, income and political conservatism. They also found that any type of contact - with a friend, a spouse, a family member, a work contact, or a contact in a public place - with mentally ill individuals reduced perceptions of dangerousness of the target in the vignette. Thus,  according to 5 the second (etc) times you cite an article, you can use et al. if the article has more than 2 authors.

Myths of violence 3

Alexander and Link (2003), any contact with the mentally ill is associated with reduced fear and rejection. However, since this study was observational in nature, we cannot know if contact reduces fear or having lower fear increased contact.

Corrigan, Rowan, Green, Lundin, River, Uphoff-Wasowski, White and Kubiak (2002) conducted two studies examining the causal processes in contact, fear and rejection. Corrigan et al. posited two models to account for stigmatizing reactions.

In the first model, labeled personal responsibility, beliefs about personality responsibility influences both the level of pity and anger displayed toward mental patients. Additionally, the variables of pity and anger influence helping behavior. In the second model, labeled dangerousness, perceived dangerousness influences fear of mental patients, which in turn influences the avoidance of the mentally ill.

In their first study, Corrigan et al. (2002) administered a questionnaire to 216 community college students. This questionnaire contained items which would allow the examination of the two models. The results of a path analysis indicated that while both models fit the data, the results for the dangerousness model seemed far more consistent with the data.

Their second study was an attempt to manipulate variables in the models. Participants met with either an educational group or with a mental patient. During the meetings, myths about the

Myths of violence 4

personality responsibility or the dangerousness of mental patients were discussed and debunked. While education yielded some positive results regarding fear and rejection, contact with mental patients produced stronger results. Thus, Corrigan et al. demonstrated that contact causes less rejection and fear.

Stigmatization of the mentally ill is caused by the public's belief in myths about the dangerousness of the mentally ill and exposing those myths can reduce stigmatization. At least one-third of the people sampled in one study said that they would both reject socially and fear violence from someone displaying behaviors associated with different mentally illnesses. Other research discovered that this rejection is
associated to lack of contact with the mentally ill and that as contact increased, fear of the mentally ill decreased. The direction of the relationship between fear and rejection seems to be that fear (possibly based upon myths about mental illness) causes rejection. Taken as a whole, it appears that exposing these myths as myths increases the acceptance of the mentally ill and that staged contact with a mentally person to expose myths has an even more powerful effect. Caution must be advised, though; Martin et al.'s (2002) and Alexander and Link's (2003) studies and the first study of Corrigan et al. (2002) were based upon paper and pencil methodologies. And while Corrigan et al.'s (2002) second study involved staged

Myths of violence 5

presentations, it was conducted in a college setting with a college sample. Future research should replicate these findings in more natural settings with different populations. 1

Myths of violence 6

References6
Alexander, L.A., & Link, B.G. (2003). The impact of contact on stigmatizing attitudes towards people with mental illness.

Journal of Mental Health, 12, 271-289.

Corrigan, P. W., Rowan, D., Green, A., Lundin, R., River, P.,

Uphoff-Wasowski, K., White, K., & Kubiak, M.A. (2002). Challenging two mental illness stigmas: Personality responsibility and dangerousness. Schizophrenia Bulletin, 28, 293-309.

Martin, J. K., Pescosolido, B. A., & Tuch, S. A. (2000). Of fear and loathing: The role of ‘disturbing behavior' labels, and causal attributions in shaping public attitudes toward people with mental illness. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 41, 208-223.

Myths of violence 7
6 references begin on a new page.

Reference no: EM131196485

Questions Cloud

Calculate the length required for a 50% coupling : Calculate the length required for a 50% coupling when the gap is 7 μD.
Why would customer choose one retail format over the others : Choose a product category that both you and your parents purchase (e.g., business clothing, casual clothing, music, electronic equipment, shampoo). In which type of store do you typically purchase this merchandise? What about your parents? Expl..
Men of passive tempers : How does Paine reject the arguments of "men of passive tempers" who wish for reconciliation with Britain?
Origins of english monarchs : What point is Paine making when he argues, "Europe, and not England, is the parent country of America"? How does he use the national origins of English monarchs to show that the colonists' English origins are irrelevant to the decision they now fa..
Brief literature review about a topic related to autism : Complete a brief literature review about a topic related to autism.- The articles used have to be peer reviewed articles.
What fraction of this machine''s production will fail : A machine produces bearings with mean diameter 3.00 inches and standard deviation 0.01 inch. Bearings with diameters in excess of 3.02 inches or less than 2.98 inches will fail to meet quality specifications.
Ways of explaining the conquest : Compare older ways of explaining the conquest (such as Moctezumas supposed belief that Hernando Cortes was a gold) with scholars more recent explanations. What beliefs about Native Americans does each set of explanations reflect?
Post the transactions to t-accounts : Selected transactions from the journal of Kati Tillman, investment broker, Instructions- Post the transactions to T-accounts. Prepare a trial balance at August 31, 2015
Would you have been ready to declare independence : Put yourself in their shoes. If you had read this (what am I talking about, of course you have READ this) back in 1776, would you have been ready to declare independence?

Reviews

Write a Review

Other Subject Questions & Answers

  What is the total profit on the combination

Sally assembles stereo equipment for resale in her shop. She offers two products turntables and cassette players. She makes a profit of $10 on each turntable and $6 on each cassette. Both must through two steps in her shop- assembly and bench checkin..

  Relationship between a paradigm and normal science

What's the relationship between a paradigm and Normal Science ? What are the scientific anormalies? Why is important to Kuhn Theory?

  Social cultural factors effect marketing strategies

How does competition, economic, political, legal, regulatory, technological, and social cultural factors effect marketing strategies?

  What should david do from a teleological perspective

From a deontological perspective, what should David do in this situation? Define deontological ethics, and then explain how your answer evolved from that definition. What should David do from a teleological perspective? Define teleological ethics ..

  Read the factual pattern and prepare the report

You are required to read the Factual Pattern. After you read the Factual Pattern, proceed to the Report. You will note that there are numbers throughout the Report. A word or phrase has to be used in place of each number to make the sentence co..

  Explain how you would deal with the situation

Explain how you would deal with the situation, if you discovered one of your colleagues was engaging in this behavior. Describe the specific steps you would take, with reference to the 2014 ACA Code of Ethics

  Victimless crime-white-collar crime and orgainzed crime

What is a victimless crime, white-collar crime, and orgainzed crime, and what are some examples of each?

  Differences between experience and that of grandparents

What does Puwat Chaukamnoetkanok see as the main differences between his experience and that of his grandparents?

  Construct three different arguments that distinct fallacies

Construct three different arguments that display distinct fallacies. Give an explanation of why each makes a mistake in drawing the conclusion it does. Review your classmates' examples and see if they, in fact, commit the fallacy identified.

  Illustrate out the socratic method

Illustrate out the Socratic method? What is it designed to do? Try to illustrate this method with a concrete example.

  Q1 the fundamental equation of a pure component is given

q1. the fundamental equation of a pure component is given byu av2expsru v and s are the molar energy volume and entropy

  International cooperative venture

An international cooperative venture involving the voluntary participation of three or more countries in an economic or political association is known as

Free Assignment Quote

Assured A++ Grade

Get guaranteed satisfaction & time on delivery in every assignment order you paid with us! We ensure premium quality solution document along with free turntin report!

All rights reserved! Copyrights ©2019-2020 ExpertsMind IT Educational Pvt Ltd